How is it possible to believe in god summary




















When asked separate questions about the roles that prayer and God play in their lives, more respondents say that God is important than say that prayer is important, though strong majorities in half of the countries surveyed say that both are important to them. Just as respondents in wealthier countries tend to disagree that it is necessary to believe in God to be a moral person, people in wealthier countries generally say that God and prayer are not especially important in their lives For more on advanced and emerging economies, see Appendix B.

People in emerging economies are more than twice as likely as people in advanced economies to agree that prayer is an important part of daily life.

Nine-in-ten or more respondents in all the emerging economies surveyed except for Ukraine say that God plays an important role in their lives. By contrast, less than half of respondents in 11 of the economically advanced countries surveyed consider God to be important in life.

In some countries, respondents are less likely to say that prayer is an important part of daily life than they are to say that God is important in their lives. Muslim Israelis drive much of this sentiment. Views on whether God plays an important role in life differ substantially based on religious affiliation as well. Still, around three-in-ten religiously unaffiliated people in Argentina and the U. There is near unanimous agreement that God is important in life among people of all major religious affiliations in Brazil, the Philippines and Kenya, as well as among all Muslim and Christian respondents in Nigeria.

Following well-documented trends that trace the decline of Christianity in Western Europe, the share of Europeans who say that God plays an important role in life has declined since Spain, Italy and Poland had the most dramatic decreases, with declines of 26, 21 and 14 percentage points, respectively.

This trend is mirrored in many other European countries, including Lithuania. Since the collapse of the USSR, Lithuania has seen a 12 percentage point drop in the share of its public that feels God plays an important role in their lives. At the same time, other former Soviet republics where religion was harshly repressed or effectively banned during the Soviet period have experienced an increase in the percentage of people who say God plays an important role in life.

Both Ukraine and Russia have experienced double-digit increases in the share of people who agree that God is important to them. It organizes the public into nine distinct groups, based on an analysis of their attitudes and values. Even in a polarized era, the survey reveals deep divisions in both partisan coalitions.

Use this tool to compare the groups on some key topics and their demographics. Pew Research Center now uses as the last birth year for Millennials in our work. President Michael Dimock explains why. About Pew Research Center Pew Research Center is a nonpartisan fact tank that informs the public about the issues, attitudes and trends shaping the world. It conducts public opinion polling, demographic research, media content analysis and other empirical social science research. Pew Research Center does not take policy positions.

It is a subsidiary of The Pew Charitable Trusts. Newsletters Donate My Account. A prior study, using some of the same measures used here, has reported that this gender difference can be explained by gender differences in mentalizing [ 8 ]. However, prior work has shown a that males have a stronger tendency towards mentalizing, and females towards empathic concern [ 26 , ].

The current findings thus raise the possibility that the gender gap in religious belief may be best explained by gender differences in empathic concern rather than gender differences in mentalizing. To assess these questions, we conducted a 3-step hierarchical linear regression using those variables common between the studies to predict religious and spiritual belief.

To address our second question, we examined both bivariate and partial correlations between belief, analytic thinking, and empathic concern. Religious and spiritual belief have been positively associated with social and emotional cognition [ 8 — 22 ] and negatively associated with measures of analytic thinking [ 1 , 4 — 8 ]. The present studies make two key contributions to the current literature.

First, we distinguish between different dimensions of social cognition, and second we assess their association with belief while also controlling for their negative association with analytic thinking.

According to our theoretical model, moral concern represents one broad dimension of social cognition distinct from mentalizing [ 24 , 29 , 30 , 46 ]. This view is supported by the observation that moral concern and mentalizing relate to distinct neuropsychological profiles, such that a deficit in moral concern is the primary personality characteristic of psychopathy [ 33 , 37 , 38 ], whereas a deficit in mentalizing is thought to be a key characteristic of Autism Spectrum Disorders [ 32 , 33 , 35 ].

To our knowledge, this is the first series of studies to simultaneously test these three cognitive constructs—analytic thinking, moral concern, and mentalizing—in order to test the extent to which each independently contributes to religious and spiritual belief.

This relationship was found to be robust even when controlling for the previously established link between analytic thinking and religious disbelief. Further, no evidence was found supporting the view that either direct or indirect measures of mentalizing predict belief after taking into account measures of moral concern. In every study we found that a central aspect of moral concern, empathic concern IRI-EC , significantly predicted religious and spiritual belief.

This relationship was found both for bivariate correlations and after entering all other variables into a regression analysis. Of the seven studies which included measures of analytic thinking, all demonstrated a negative relationship with belief in the bivariate correlations; however, this relationship only remained significant in 2 out of the 7 studies after entering other variables into the regression analyses.

We conducted a pooled analysis to establish whether or not a measure of analytic thinking CRT remained significant after accounting for its negative relationship with empathic concern IRI-EC.

We found that CRT performance did account for variance in belief; however, the effect was significantly smaller than the positive relationship observed with the IRI-EC. Four of the eight studies included measures of mentalizing.

We used 3 direct self-report measures of mentalizing, 5 direct objective or performance based measures of mentalizing, and one indirect self-report measure loneliness. None of the four studies demonstrated a positive relationship between belief and any of the nine measures of mentalizing after other variables were taken into account in the regression analysis.

These findings suggest that any positive relationship with mentalizing is considerably weaker than the positive relationship with moral concern. Further, it appears that some measures of mentalizing associated with the Autistic phenotype AQ and EQ may have previously been interpreted as demonstrating an association with belief because they fail to fully distinguish mentalizing from moral concern.

This is suggested by our findings in Studies 5 and 6 in which AQ and EQ demonstrated relationships with belief in the bivariate correlations but did not survive in the regression analyses after measures of moral concern were taken into account.

Hence, the current results suggest that non-believers have personality profiles more closely associated with the psychopathic phenotype i. The relationships observed between belief, analytic thinking, mentalizing and moral concern fit well with a model we have previously presented and supported with neuroscientific evidence [ 24 , 29 , 31 , , ].

According to that model, analytic thinking and moral concern represent two cognitive modes which our neural architecture causes to be in competition with each other. While the present studies have provided good support for hypotheses generated by our model, they are limited in a number of ways. First, since we rely exclusively on correlational observations, further work is needed to better establish causal relationships between belief, analytic thinking and moral concern, especially when investigated simultaneously.

It has been shown that priming analytic thinking reduces religious belief [ 4 , 5 ]. In addition, it has been shown that priming religious and spiritual thinking increases prosocial behavior [ ]. However, we are unaware of any studies which have manipulated both, or which have included measures to assess whether the observed effects are mediated by the hypothesized competitive relationship between analytic thinking and moral concern.

Second, while we employed performance based measures of mentalizing, the measures of moral concern were all subjective either self or peer-report. More objective measures of moral concern are wanted to further establish their relationship to religious and spiritual belief. Third, although we assess the impact of analytic thinking and moral concern while controlling for measures of the other variable, it is unlikely that we were able to capture all the variance in either analytic thinking or moral concern using our control variables.

Hence, for example, it remains possible that the observed relationship between analytic thinking and belief might disappear after fully controlling for moral concern by using a variety of measures. Again, better and more comprehensive measures of moral concern are wanted. Fourth, the distinct dimensions of social cognition remain to be fully explored. The present findings strongly indicate that measures of moral concern, broadly construed, are better predictors of belief than measures of mentalizing, broadly construed.

However, it is not safe to conclude that moral concern is the only or best predictor of religious belief. Further work is needed to subdivide social cognition and establish relationships between dimensions; and also to examine the relationships between distinct dimensions of social cognition and distinct dimensions of non-social reasoning including but not limited to analytic thinking.

There are worries that this compromises the validity of the measures, especially the CRT. We did not ask the participants whether they had seen these measures before. Hence, it is possible that familiarity with some measures may have biased results. In this respect, it is worth noting that these studies were conducted between and , when researchers were just beginning to use the AMT. Finally, the validity of our findings are further supported by the correspondence between our AMT studies and our in lab study Study 5.

The results reported here are also highly consistent with overlapping findings reported by other researchers employing both AMT and laboratory methods [ 4 — 6 ]. A prior study found that belief in a personal god was negatively related to measures of the ASD phenotype and that this relationship was mediated by deficits in mentalizing [ 8 ]. In that study, mentalizing was assessed by the empathy quotient EQ and reading the mind in the eyes task, both of which were used in the studies reported here.

While we did find a positive relationship between belief and EQ in Study 6, the relationship was no longer significant in the regression analysis after taking into account analytic thinking and moral concern.

In Study 5, we did not replicate the previously observed relationship [ 8 ] between belief and Reading the Mind in the Eyes. It is possible that we did not have the power to detect this relationship. Nevertheless, our data provide evidence that moral concern predicts belief more strongly than previously used measures of mentalizing. It is thus possible that the nature of religious belief e. In support of this, other work suggests that the specific form of religious belief e.

Here, we focused on associations between moral concern and religious and spiritual belief more broadly. A second inconsistency concerns the results of Study 7, where we investigated links between loneliness and belief. Prior work has provided evidence that loneliness increases the perception of agency in inanimate objects and belief in supernatural agents [ 99 ]. Correlational evidence showed that higher levels of loneliness increased the tendency to anthropomorphize inanimate gadgets.

What might explain the inconsistencies between these prior findings and the present work? First, it is worth noting that the previously reported correlational evidence only provides support for a relationship between loneliness and anthropomorphizing inanimate objects correlations between loneliness and religious belief were not tested.

Second, it is likely that their loneliness induction, which was used to investigate religious belief, had non-specific effects. The induction may have not only encouraged feelings of loneliness, but also feelings of moral concern. It would be beneficial for future work to control for moral concern when conducting experimental manipulations designed to investigate the cognitive origins of religious and spiritual beliefs.

These results reported here present a challenge to a number of theoretical accounts of religious belief, especially those which emphasize a link between religious and spiritual beliefs and the perception of agency [ 8 , 11 , 16 , 17 , 22 ].

The present findings put religious and spiritual beliefs in a new light by suggesting that they are not so much linked to the perception of agency as they are broadly to moral concern, and in particular empathic concern.

In line with this view, a number of theologians and religious scholars have claimed that compassion is a central theme that unites many religions [ , ]. While further work is needed to establish causal links, it is plausible both that religious thinking increases moral concern, and that individuals who possess greater levels of moral concern are more inclined to identify with religious and spiritual worldviews. Performed the experiments: JPF.

Browse Subject Areas? Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field. Abstract Prior work has established that analytic thinking is associated with disbelief in God, whereas religious and spiritual beliefs have been positively linked to social and emotional cognition.

Funding: The authors have no support or funding to report. Introduction In the last decade, a number of theorists and experimentalists have attempted to address the question of what psychological mechanisms give rise to religious and spiritual beliefs.

Further, there is empirical support for the view that moral concern for others can lead to aggression in the context of perceived threat [ 48 ] Our theory, the opposing domains hypothesis [ 24 , 29 , 30 ], holds that our neural architecture has evolved in such a way that it creates a tension between analytic thinking and moral concern.

Study 2 Study 2 aimed to extend the results of Study 1 by including the item Mill-Hill Vocabulary scale, which we used as a measure of crystallized intelligence CI [ 89 ]. Study 3 This study recruited a sample of adults from the USA from AMT in order to replicate and extend our previous results by including an additional measure of moral concern.

Study 4 Study 4 recruited a sample of adults from the USA from AMT to test the prediction that, once we control for measures of empathic concern and analytic thinking, a measure of mentalizing does not add any predictive power to the regression model. Study 5 Study 5 extends Study 4 beyond self-report measures to also include a peer reported measure of empathy [ 93 ] and objective e. Participants completed these measures over several different sessions in the laboratory.

Study 6 Study 6 extended the assessment of the ASD phenotype using two other measures previously investigated alongside belief [ 8 ]: the empathizing quotient EQ and the systemizing quotient SQ [ 27 ]. Study 7 Studies 4—6 used direct measures of mentalizing, and found no positive association with religious and spiritual belief. Study 8 The prior seven studies provide compelling evidence that religious belief is positively associated with various measures of empathy and negatively associated with analytic thinking.

Pooled Analysis Prior work demonstrates a small but consistent gender gap in religious belief, with females tending to hold more to religious and spiritual worldviews [ ]. Given inconsistent findings concerning the relationship between analytic thinking and belief, what are the relative magnitudes of the associations with empathic concern and analytic thinking?

General Discussion Religious and spiritual belief have been positively associated with social and emotional cognition [ 8 — 22 ] and negatively associated with measures of analytic thinking [ 1 , 4 — 8 ]. Limitations While the present studies have provided good support for hypotheses generated by our model, they are limited in a number of ways.

Inconsistencies with Prior Work A prior study found that belief in a personal god was negatively related to measures of the ASD phenotype and that this relationship was mediated by deficits in mentalizing [ 8 ].

Broader Significance These results reported here present a challenge to a number of theoretical accounts of religious belief, especially those which emphasize a link between religious and spiritual beliefs and the perception of agency [ 8 , 11 , 16 , 17 , 22 ].

Supporting Information. S1 Data. S1 Supporting Information. References 1. Average intelligence predicts atheism rates across nations. View Article Google Scholar 2.

Norenzayan A, Gervais WM. The origins of religious disbelief. Trends Cogn Sci. Nyborg H. The intelligence—religiosity nexus: A representative study of white adolescent Americans. View Article Google Scholar 4. Divine intuition: cognitive style influences belief in God.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. View Article Google Scholar 5. Gervais WM, Norenzayan A. Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. The relation between intelligence and religiosity a meta-analysis and some proposed explanations.

Personality and Social Psychology Review. View Article Google Scholar 8. Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal God. PLoS One. Prosocial behavior and religion: New evidence based on projective measures and peer ratings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. View Article Google Scholar Banerjee K, Bloom P. Would Tarzan believe in God? Conditions for the emergence of religious belief. Trends in cognitive sciences.

Barrett JL. Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Why would anyone believe in God? Bering J. Bering JM. The existential theory of mind.

Review of General Psychology. Bloom P. Religion is natural. Developmental science. Boyer P. Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought: Basic Books; Gervais WM.

Perspect Psychol Sci. Religion replenishes self-control. Psychological Science. Personal prayer buffers self-control depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. Liu C-C. The relationship between personal religious orientation and emotional intelligence. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal. Paek E. Religiosity and perceived emotional intelligence among Christians.

Personality and individual differences. Dennett DC. Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural phenomenon: Penguin; Dissecting the social brain: Introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain—behavior relations for empathy and Theory of Mind. Confess your sin to the Lord and repent of whatever you did wrong. No one is born perfect and God will always love you.

Not Helpful 38 Helpful My dad had a bad experience with the Christian faith as a child and now he doesn't want me to believe in God. I'm forced to practice my faith alone and I can't go to church. What do I do? Establish regular prayer in your life, which means daily talk to God and pour out your worries and praise for all he provides. This can be done anywhere you can find a quiet place.

Understand that Abraham had to undergo this trial when his own father refused to accept faith in God. Know that a relationship with God is personal and with him as your friend you are never alone in this world. God is our creator, protector, sustainer, provider, and guardian. Only he can do these things, and only he hears and answers our prayers and deserves our praise.

Not Helpful 8 Helpful Ritual may help you get into a mental state where you can more easily connect with the divine, but the trappings are not necessary and your faith is not any better or worse than that of someone who does all of the "right" steps.

Your love and curiosity is all you need to bring. Not Helpful 11 Helpful Yes, of course! God tells us in the Bible that it's not our position to judge, so loving the LGBT community is a truly Christian thing to do.

How will I ever see what God wants me to do if I'll never be touched by the holy spirit? You just have to pray and during that time of prayer, ask what God wants you to do and listen to God's voice. You could feel a tugging in your heart or a powerful urge to do something during that time.

Follow that urge and it should lead you in the right direction. Also, if you believe in God, desire to do good, have a personal relationship with God, and are willing to fill yourself up with God, then the Holy Spirit will be always be there to guide you. Don't ever lose hope or patience. Another good way to find what God wants you to do is to participate in your church's ministries and volunteer for charity groups.

Include your email address to get a message when this question is answered. If things seem desperate, keep at it. You have a purpose and God knows it! Helpful 2 Not Helpful 0. Do not give up your faith, because some challenge comes along. When it knocks you down to your knees, look upward and pray. God had a reason for allowing free will and choice.

We are not robots nor programmed by instincts or unalterable drives as animals. When you seek Him early you'll find Him. A door will be opened. When God closes one door; he opens another one Helpful 3 Not Helpful 0. Everything in life, every path you take, you have taken for a reason, if you are following God's will.

Write it down, and follow the path. Then one day, read that Book, and track that path you took. Understand how the first path led to an old way, a straight path. When you find faith, hold on to it tightly; do not allow it to slip away; do not stop believing. One day you may understand the essence of knowing, I have a purpose in life , and if you are still seeking you can find greater purpose, perhaps even when you least expect it.

Have faith. Do not become weary in well doing nor fall back. Believe, and you will never be alone. You do not have to believe or join any particular religion to have faith in God. Go to a website that tells you more about your need of God, and begin a new life with God today. Helpful 1 Not Helpful 0. If loved ones die, and you ask "Why? A reason can be shown to you in time. Until then, remember to " This article applies to the personal God and presupposes your own and God's existence is necessary and purposeful.

Though different faiths profess different views of God, they transcend our ideas of any being, whether male or female: God is greater than this Many say that "seeing is believing", but is that so with God? If you say "I'm a Christian. Jesus said. The finely tuned design gave intelligence, physical control, senses and emotions -- that point to organized, purposeful traits not random with consequences and rewards now and later. Learn about personal testimonies from people whose lives were saved or transformed by their faith in God.

Submit a Tip All tip submissions are carefully reviewed before being published. People will disagree with you many times, don't make a big deal out of it.

Respect other people's religions, they believe differently than you do. That's for each person to decide. That's okay. Helpful Not Helpful Related wikiHows How to. How to. More References 8.

Co-authors: Updated: April 29, Categories: Religion. Italiano: Credere in Dio. Deutsch: An Gott glauben. Bahasa Indonesia: Percaya pada Tuhan. Thanks to all authors for creating a page that has been read , times. Did this article help you? Cookies make wikiHow better. By continuing to use our site, you agree to our cookie policy. About This Article Co-authors:



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000